Developmental Science,
Journal Year:
2021,
Volume and Issue:
25(1)
Published: July 8, 2021
Abstract
Parents
with
fewer
educational
and
economic
resources
(low
socioeconomic‐status,
SES)
tend
to
speak
less
their
children,
consequences
for
children's
later
life
outcomes.
Despite
this
well‐established
highly
popularized
link,
research
addresses
why
the
SES
“word
gap”
exists.
Moreover,
while
has
assessed
individual‐level
contributors
word
gap—like
differences
in
parenting
knowledge—we
know
little
about
how
structural
constraints
that
vary
according
might
affect
caregivers’
speech.
In
two
pre‐registered
studies,
we
test
whether
experiencing
financial
scarcity
can
suppress
speech
children.
Study
1
suggests
higher‐SES
caregivers
who
are
prompted
reflect
on
scarcity—particularly
those
scarcity—speak
3‐year‐olds
a
subsequent
play
session,
relative
control
group.
2
mid‐
engage
back‐and‐forth
exchanges
children
at
end
of
month—when
they
more
likely
be
hardship—than
rest
month.
These
studies
provide
preliminary
evidence
that—above
beyond
individual
characteristics—structural
may
much
parents
Nature reviews. Neuroscience,
Journal Year:
2021,
Volume and Issue:
22(6), P. 372 - 384
Published: April 28, 2021
Childhood
socio-economic
status
(SES),
a
measure
of
the
availability
material
and
social
resources,
is
one
strongest
predictors
lifelong
well-being.
Here
we
review
evidence
that
experiences
associated
with
childhood
SES
affect
not
only
outcome
but
also
pace
brain
development.
We
argue
higher
protracted
structural
development
prolonged
trajectory
functional
network
segregation,
ultimately
leading
to
more
efficient
cortical
networks
in
adulthood.
hypothesize
greater
exposure
chronic
stress
accelerates
maturation,
whereas
access
novel
positive
decelerates
maturation.
discuss
impact
variation
on
plasticity
learning.
provide
generative
theoretical
framework
catalyse
future
basic
science
translational
research
environmental
influences
Evidence
suggests
can
its
rate.
Tooley,
Bassett
Mackey
this
suggest
valence
frequency
early
interact
influence
JAMA Pediatrics,
Journal Year:
2019,
Volume and Issue:
174(1), P. e193869 - e193869
Published: Nov. 4, 2019
The
American
Academy
of
Pediatrics
(AAP)
recommends
limits
on
screen-based
media
use,
citing
its
cognitive-behavioral
risks.
Screen
use
by
young
children
is
prevalent
and
increasing,
although
implications
for
brain
development
are
unknown.To
explore
the
associations
between
integrity
white
matter
tracts
supporting
language
literacy
skills
in
preschool-aged
children.This
cross-sectional
study
healthy
aged
3
to
5
years
(n
=
47)
was
conducted
from
August
2017
November
2018.
Participants
were
recruited
at
a
US
children's
hospital
community
primary
care
clinics.Children
completed
cognitive
testing
followed
diffusion
tensor
imaging
(DTI),
their
parent
ScreenQ
survey.ScreenQ
15-item
measure
reflecting
domains
AAP
recommendations:
access
screens,
frequency
content
viewed,
coviewing.
Higher
scores
reflect
greater
use.
applied
as
independent
variable
multiple
linear
regression
models,
with
standardized
assessments
dependent
variable,
controlling
child
age
household
income:
Comprehensive
Test
Phonological
Processing,
Second
Edition
(CTOPP-2;
Rapid
Object
Naming
subtest);
Expressive
Vocabulary
Test,
(EVT-2;
expressive
language);
Get
Ready
Read!
(GRTR;
emergent
skills).
DTI
measures
included
fractional
anisotropy
(FA)
radial
diffusivity
(RD),
which
estimated
microstructural
organization
myelination
tracts.
factor
associated
FA
RD
whole-brain
analyses,
then
narrowed
left-sided
abilities.Of
69
recruited,
47
(among
whom
27
[57%]
girls,
mean
[SD]
54.3
[7.5]
months)
DTI.
Mean
(SD;
range)
score
8.6
(4.8;
1-19)
points.
CTOPP-2
9.4
(3.3;
2-15)
points,
EVT-2
113.1
(16.6;
88-144)
GRTR
19.0
(5.9;
5-25)
negatively
correlated
(F2,43
5.14;
R2
0.19;
P
<
.01),
(F2,35
6.64;
0.28;
(F2,44
17.08;
0.44;
.01)
scores,
age.
lower
higher
involved
language,
executive
function,
abilities
(P
.05,
familywise
error-corrected),
income.This
found
an
association
increased
compared
guidelines,
prekindergarten
children.
findings
suggest
further
needed,
particularly
during
rapid
early
stages
development.
Perspectives on Psychological Science,
Journal Year:
2021,
Volume and Issue:
16(6), P. 1463 - 1472
Published: Sept. 7, 2021
We
review
the
three
prevailing
approaches—specificity,
cumulative
risk,
and
dimensional
models—to
conceptualizing
developmental
consequences
of
early-life
adversity
address
fundamental
problems
with
characterization
these
frameworks
in
a
recent
Perspectives
on
Psychological
Science
piece
by
Smith
Pollak.
respond
to
concerns
raised
Pollak
about
models
early
experience
highlight
value
for
studying
adversity.
Basic
dimensions
proposed
existing
include
threat/harshness,
deprivation,
unpredictability.
These
identify
core
that
cut
across
categorical
exposures
have
been
focus
specificity
risk
approaches
(e.g.,
abuse,
institutional
rearing,
chronic
poverty);
delineate
aspects
are
likely
influence
brain
behavioral
development;
afford
hypotheses
adaptive
maladaptive
responses
different
adversity;
articulate
specific
mechanisms
through
which
exert
their
influences,
experience-driven
plasticity
within
an
evolutionary-developmental
framework.
In
doing
so,
advance
falsifiable
hypotheses,
grounded
neurodevelopmental
evolutionary
principles,
supported
accumulating
evidence
provide
fertile
ground
empirical
studies
Development and Psychopathology,
Journal Year:
2022,
Volume and Issue:
34(2), P. 447 - 471
Published: March 14, 2022
Abstract
Two
extant
frameworks
–
the
harshness-unpredictability
model
and
threat-deprivation
attempt
to
explain
which
dimensions
of
adversity
have
distinct
influences
on
development.
These
models
address,
respectively,
why,
based
a
history
natural
selection,
development
operates
way
it
does
across
range
environmental
contexts,
how
neural
mechanisms
that
underlie
plasticity
learning
in
response
experiences
influence
brain
Building
these
frameworks,
we
advance
an
integrated
experience,
focusing
threat-based
forms
harshness,
deprivation-based
unpredictability.
This
makes
clear
why
are
inextricable
and,
together,
essential
understanding
environment
matter.
Core
integrative
concepts
include
directedness
learning,
multiple
levels
developmental
adaptation
environment,
tradeoffs
between
adaptive
maladaptive
responses
adversity.
The
proposes
proximal
distal
cues
as
well
unpredictability
those
cues,
calibrate
both
immediate
rearing
environments
broader
ecological
current
future.
We
highlight
actionable
directions
for
research
needed
investigate
experience.
Child Development,
Journal Year:
2021,
Volume and Issue:
92(2), P. 484 - 501
Published: Jan. 31, 2021
This
meta-analysis
examined
associations
between
the
quantity
and
quality
of
parental
linguistic
input
children's
language.
Pooled
effect
size
for
(i.e.,
vocabulary
diversity
syntactic
complexity;
k
=
35;
N
1,958;
r
.33)
was
more
robust
than
number
words/tokens/utterances;
33;
1,411;
.20)
input.
For
input,
sizes
were
stronger
when
observed
in
naturalistic
contexts
compared
to
free
play
tasks.
also
increased
as
child
age
observation
length
increased.
Effect
not
moderated
by
socioeconomic
status
or
gender.
Findings
highlight
a
key
environmental
factor
language
skills.
JAMA Pediatrics,
Journal Year:
2024,
Volume and Issue:
178(4), P. 369 - 369
Published: March 4, 2024
Growing
up
in
a
language-rich
home
environment
is
important
for
children's
language
development
the
early
years.
The
concept
of
"technoference"
(technology-based
interference)
suggests
that
screen
time
may
be
interfering
with
opportunities
talk
and
interactions
between
parent
child;
however,
limited
longitudinal
evidence
exists
exploring
this
association.
Child Development,
Journal Year:
2019,
Volume and Issue:
91(3), P. 846 - 860
Published: March 28, 2019
The
mechanisms
underlying
socioeconomic
disparities
in
children's
reading
skills
are
not
well
understood.
This
study
examined
associations
among
background,
home
linguistic
input,
brain
structure,
and
5‐to‐9‐year‐old
children
(
N
=
94).
Naturalistic
audio
recordings
high‐resolution,
T1‐weighted
MRI
scans
were
acquired.
Children
who
experienced
more
adult–child
conversational
turns
or
adult
words
had
greater
left
perisylvian
cortical
surface
area.
Language
input
mediated
the
association
between
parental
education
was
indirectly
associated
with
via
Left
area
skills.
experience
may
thus
partially
explain
language‐supporting
structure
turn
Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence,
Journal Year:
2019,
Volume and Issue:
33(01), P. 687 - 694
Published: July 17, 2019
Personalized
education
technologies
capable
of
delivering
adaptive
interventions
could
play
an
important
role
in
addressing
the
needs
diverse
young
learners
at
a
critical
time
school
readiness.
We
present
innovative
personalized
social
robot
learning
companion
system
that
utilizes
children’s
verbal
and
nonverbal
affective
cues
to
modulate
their
engagement
maximize
long-term
gains.
propose
reinforcement
approach
train
policy
for
each
student
during
educational
activity
where
child
tell
stories
other.
Using
policy,
selects
are
optimized
child’s
linguistic
skill
progression.
recruited
67
bilingual
English
language
between
ages
4–6
years
old
participate
between-subjects
study
evaluate
our
system.
Over
three-month
deployment
schools,
unique
storytelling
was
trained
deliver
story
curriculum
group.
compared
outcomes
Non-personalized
group
with
fixed
robot,
baseline
had
no
intervention.
In
Personalization
condition,
results
show
successfully
boost
respect
retaining
more
target
words
as
well
using
syntax
structures
children
other
groups.
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research,
Journal Year:
2020,
Volume and Issue:
63(4), P. 1093 - 1105
Published: April 17, 2020
Purpose
The
Language
Environment
Analysis
(LENA)
system
provides
automated
measures
facilitating
clinical
and
nonclinical
research
interventions
on
language
development,
but
there
are
only
a
few,
scattered
independent
reports
of
these
measures'
validity.
objectives
the
current
systematic
review
were
to
(a)
discover
studies
comparing
LENA
output
with
manual
annotation,
namely,
accuracy
talker
labels,
as
well
involving
adult
word
counts
(AWCs),
conversational
turn
(CTCs),
child
vocalization
(CVCs);
(b)
describe
them
qualitatively;
(c)
quantitatively
integrate
assess
central
tendencies;
(d)
potential
moderators.
Method
Searches
Google
Scholar,
PubMed,
Scopus,
PsycInfo
combined
expert
knowledge,
interarticle
citations
resulting
in
238
records
screened
73
whose
full
text
was
inspected.
To
be
included,
must
target
children
under
age
18
years
report
labels
(e.g.,
precision
and/or
recall)
AWC,
CTC,
or
CVC
(correlations
error
metrics).
Results
A
total
33
studies,
28
articles,
discovered.
qualitative
revealed
most
validation
had
not
been
peer
reviewed
such
failed
key
methodology
results.
Quantitative
integration
results
possible
for
broad
definition
recall
(
M
=
59%
68%,
respectively;
N
12–13),
AWC
(mean
r
.79,
13),
.77,
5),
CTC
.36,
6).
Publication
bias
moderators
could
assessed
meta-analytically.
Conclusion
Further
improved
reporting
needed
evaluating
segmentation
quantification
accuracy,
work
investigating
being
particularly
urgent.
Supplemental
Material
https://osf.io/4nhms/