Loading...

This article is published under a Creative Commons license and not by the author of the article. So if you find any inaccuracies, you can correct them by updating the article.

Loading...
Loading...

Predictors of Turkish Elementary Teacher Candidates’ Energy Conservation Behaviors: An Approach on Value-Belief-Norm Theory Creative Commons

Link for citation this article

Elvan Sahin

Interdisciplinary Journal of Environmental and Science Education, Journal Year: 2013, Volume and Issue: 8(2), P. 269 - 283

Published: April 10, 2013

Latest article update: Jan. 10, 2023

This article is published under the license

License
Link for citation this article Related Articles

Abstract

The present study aimed to explain elementary teacher candidates’ energy conservation behaviors by using Value-Belief-Norm (VBN) Theory. Participants in this study were 512 students at Faculty of Education from two public universities in Turkey. Of the 512 students, 35.5% were enrolled in the early childhood education program, 30.9% were in the elementary science education program, and 27.7% were in the elementary mathematics education program. The rest of the participants were pursuing a graduate program under the department of elementary education. The results of multiple linear regression analysis reflected that VBN Theory could successfully explain the participants’ energy conservation behaviors. The teacher candidates’ reduction in energy consumption could be accounted by personal norms, egoistic and biospheric value orientations. However, rather than personal norms, egoistic and biospheric value orientations made greater unique contribution to explain the criterion variable. Furthermore, each predictor variable in the causal model of VBN Theory was found to be significantly associated with the next variable, but value orientations deserved more attention than VBN Theory places on them within the study context.

Keywords

Energy conservation, Value-Belief-Norm Theory, teacher candidates

Introduction


It was reported that in 1970, only 51.5% of the population had an access to electricity in Turkey but by 1987, this ratio raised up to 99.7% which indicated electricity benefit for considerably huge amount of Turkish population (Dilaver, 2009). In the early years of that period, lightening was the major intent for electricity consumption by householders. However, new appliances such as washing machine, refrigerator, and television entered the daily life of householders along with an increase in family income and technological development. As a result, household electricity demand for Turkish residential sector emerged as a significant shareholder in overall electricity consumption. The critical issue underlying this progress is that household energy consumption significantly raises greenhouse gas emissions. Being aware of the potential consequences of climate change, Turkish government ratified the Kyoto Protocol in 2009, which is an international agreement to reduce worldwide greenhouse gas emissions. However, energy consumption of Turkish population increased by about 9.8% in 2010 when compared to the year of 2009 by using natural gas and fossil fuels which are responsible for greenhouse gas emissions (Ministry of Energy and Natural Resources, 2011).


In recent years, some adaptation programs also have been proposed to cope with climate change (United Nations Development Program [UNDP], 2010). However, behavioral challenges are among the systematic barriers to climate change adaptation. While suggesting some strategies as a remedy to climate change, barrier removal activities covering enhancement of awareness, education and training at individual and community levels to build capacity and have an impact on behavioral transformations have been argued in the adaptation programs (United Nations Development Program [UNDP], 2010). Thus, it could be inferred that education for sustainable development (ESD) is a key agent within the climate change adaptation and reduction in energy use.


Education for sustainable development in higher education sector has been advocated to make a major contribution to overcome the challenges of sustainability issues such as climate change, species loss, energy use and supply (Haigh, 2005). The universities should provide highly skilled individuals necessary for every labor market such as teachers, engineers, architects, scientists, and many more. Among these professionals, teachers should be well equipped with the competences to educate the next generations, and make important decisions which affect entire societies. More specifically, the graduates of teacher education programs have a crucially important responsibility in preparing pupils to become active citizens toward a better and self-sustainable world (Summers, Comey and Childs, 2004). Realizing social and cultural characteristics of Turkish society with a large population of young people, it is an urgent need to enable young people to cultivate awareness, values, and skills to create a sustainable future. At this point, it should be noted that education for sustainable development has been arriving into all levels of education including early childhood, elementary and high school education as well as higher education (Webster, 2004). In this context, education for sustainable development in the teacher education programs could not discard to prepare future teachers to act as role models about sustainability related issues including energy conservation and efficiency. Since young people are educated through the guidance of teachers, future teachers should be well equipped with the necessary affections, awareness, values and beliefs in order to overcome the obstacles toward a sustainable world (Alkis & Ozturk, 2007).


In this context, the present study aimed to determine which psychological attributes were associated with teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors. With reference to Stem and colleagues' (1999), Value-Belief-Norm (VBN) Theory proposed to explain pro- environmental behaviors guided this study. As suggested by Steg, Dreijerink, and Abrahamse (2005), the complete VBN Theory was used to explain teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors.


Theoretical Framework


Considering the field of environmental psychology and environmental education research, considerable attempts have been made to clarify the factors pertaining to individuals' engagement in a particular pro-environmental
behavior by paying attention to socio- psychological theories. The literature review pointed out special attention paid on the theories of attitude-behavior connection in the context of environmental education research. However, as reported by Kollmus and Agyeman (2002), a varying and usually very small explanatory power of attitude as well as knowledge was shown for pro-environmental behaviors. These authors also advocated that the gap among knowledge, attitude, and behavior was unexpected because there was a common assumption on dependency of individuals' life preferences on their attitudes, beliefs, and values. Early simplistic linear models of pro- environmental behaviors were proposed based on the mentioned assumption. In detail, it was hypothesized that more knowledge and favorable attitudes toward the environment would lead to pro-environmental behaviors. On the other hand, Kollmus and Agyeman (2002) concluded that the reliance on enhancement of environmental knowledge and attitude to facilitate behavioral change is surprising since such a transition is very difficult. In this aspect, Rajecki (1982) outlined the possible causes of attitude-behavior discrepancy and emphasized the complexity of designing valid research studies that measure and compare attitude and behavior. The Theory of Reasoned Action and the Theory of Planned Behavior were proposed by Ajzen and Fishbein to overcome such measurement discrepancies (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). Thus, some research studies (e.g Boldero, 1995; Kurz, Linden, & Sheehy, 2007; Sparks and Shepherd, 1992; Taylor & Todd, 1995a, 1995b) focused on the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991) deduced from the Theory of Reasoned Action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980) to grasp the nature of environment-related behaviors.


The Theory of Planned Behavior, being one of the fundamental behavioral models postulates that human behavior results from three belief constructs; beliefs about the likely consequences of a particular behavior and the evaluations of these consequences (behavioral beliefs), beliefs about the normative expectations of important others (e.g parents, friends, neighbours, teachers) and motivation to comply with these expectations (normative beliefs), and beliefs about the presence of factors that may support or prevent performance of the behavior and perceived significance of these factors (control beliefs). With their respective incorporation, behavioral beliefs result in a favorable or an unfavorable attitude toward the behavior; normative beliefs yield perceived social pressure or subjective norm; and control beliefs produce perceived behavioral control. In aggregates, attitude toward the behavior, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control give rise to the formation of an intention to perform the behavior. It is of importance to note that intention is supposed to be the immediate antecedent of human behavior. Finally, proposed by Banter (2005), the adaptive ability of the Theory of Planned Behavior reflecting any changes in content, context and environment facilitates validity and applicability of this model.


On contrary, Value-Belief-Norm Theory, which is an inclusion of values, awareness of consequences, ascription of responsibility, and personal norms, grew out of value theory (Schwartz, 1992, 1994; Stem & Dietz, 1994), the New Environmental Paradigm (NEP), and Norm- Activation Model (NAM). Regarding the significant determinants of proenvironmental behaviors, some researchers (Karp, 1996; Nordlund, & Garvill, 2002; Sener, & Hazer, 2007; Stem, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993; Van Vugt, Meertens, & Van Lange, 1995) insisted on valuebasis of a specific behavior such as energy conservation and preferences on travel mode. This theoretical perspective emerged depending on the following point of view: some proenvironmental behaviors which necessitate short-time sacrifices require an individual to suppress egoistic motives for the sake of collective interests (Steg, Dreijerink, & Abrahamse, 2005). Some researchers (Stem & Dietz, 1994; Stem, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993) showed that values oriented on different aspects influence an individual's willingness to take an active role in environmental protection. Specifically, environmental concern is associated with valued objects which are oriented around self, other people, or nonhuman species and the biosphere. In egoistic value orientation emphasis is placed on individual outcomes (Stem & Dietz, 1994). Egoistic individuals are concerned about the environment for the sake of their own interests and desires. On contrary, social-altruistic value orientation points out an overall concern for the welfare of other human beings. Behaviors of individuals with social-altruistic value orientations are determined by their concerns about environmental problems due to their adverse effect on other people. However, biospheric value orientation focuses on


nonhuman species or the biosphere and such individuals are concerned about all living things including plants and animals (Schultz et al., 2005). Value orientations have been considered in the context of clusters on universal human values proposed by Schwartz (1992, 1994). The clusters ‘conservation' and ‘self-enhancement' reflect an egoistic value orientation, while the clusters ‘self-transcendence' and ‘openness to change' reflect biospheric-altruistic value orientations. Empirical research (Guiterrez Karp, 1996; Schultz & Zelezny, 1999) carried out to investigate the role of value orientations on predicting pro-environmental behaviors pointed out that individuals with more favorable biospheric and altruistic value orientations had higher tendency to demonstrate such kind of behaviors.


Apart from the value theory, The New Environmental Paradigm (Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, and Jones, 2000) is a worldview supposing that environmentally responsible behaviors result from general beliefs on the interaction between human and nature. Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, and Jones (2000, p. 427) stated that ‘New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) focused on beliefs about humanity's ability to upset the balance of nature, the existence of limits to growth for human societies, and humanity's right to rule over the rest of nature ’. In the context of VBN theory, the NEP is interpreted as a representative agent of pro-environmental general beliefs and a conducive to demonstrate some behaviors for the sake of nature conservation (Dunlap et al. 2000; Menzel & Bogeholz, 2010; Schultz & Zelezny, 1999). Dunlap et al. (2000) also reported that in spite of the difficulty of explaining pro-environmental behaviors by using general beliefs, various studies could show significant associations between the NEP and some kinds of behavioral intentions as well as self-reported and observed behaviors. For instance, Menzel and Bogeholz (2010) examining pupils' commitment to protect biodiversity reported that the NEP could be regarded as a positive predictor of this behavioral type in the German sample and emphasized the significance of beliefs on human-nature interdependence in educational programmes. On the other hand, strong relationship between worldviews and pro-environmental behaviors could not be found in some research studies (e.g.Poortinga, Steg & Vick, 2004; Vining & Ebreo, 1992). This result has been attributed to the fact that behavior-specific beliefs may act as a mediator between worldviews and behaviors.


As the third baseline of VBN theory, Norm- Activation Model (Schwartz, 1977; Schwartz and Howard, 1981) was developed to predict the role of personal norms and moral values on altruistic behavior in humans; a type of behavior originating from the interest of others rather than of the self. In addition to altruistic behavior, this model has often been applied to explain pro- environmental behavior (e.g. Guagnano, Dietz, & Stem, 1994; Stem, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993; Vining & Ebreo, 1992). The NAM showing a moral approach stresses that pro-environmental behavior is more likely to be observed if people feel a moral obligation to perform the behavior (Harland and Wilke, 2007). Such a feeling of moral obligation to perform the behavior was categorized as ‘personal norms', and Harland and Wilke (2007) portrayed that the NAM is associated with a complex model of human decision making in moral situations. In addition to an emphasis on personal norms in the context of the NAM, the model suggests that the individual should be also aware of the adverse consequences of this kind of behaviors to the environment or others (awareness of consequences), and should possess feelings of responsibility for causing or preventing these adverse consequences (ascription of responsibility). The attribute of personal norms has been regarded as the substantial factor within the framework of the considered theory. The NAM was advocated to be powerful in explaining low-cost pro-environmental behaviors, but less successful when there are strong constraints (i.e effort, time, money) on the related behavior (Bamberg & Schmidt, 2003; Hunecke, Bldbaum, Matthies, and Hoger, 2001).


Considering the above mentioned perspectives used to predict pro-environmental behaviors, VBN theory proposes that awareness of consequences (AC) and ascribed responsibility (AR) are linked to general beliefs on humannature interdependence (NEP) and value orientations clustered as egoism, biospherism, and altruism. Furthermore, personal norms regarded as the ultimate predictor of behaviors could be activated by these environment-related attributes. Figure 1 presents the causal chain that could be examined within VBN Theory.



Stem and colleagues (Stem et al., 1999) classified the types of behaviors that personal norms may have a direct impact on: environmental activism (e.g. participating in public demonstrations, active involvement in environmental organizations), nonactivist behaviors in the public sphere (e.g. signing a petition for nature conservation, voting a pro- environmental candidate), private sphere behaviors (e.g. purchasing environmental- friendly products, reductions in energy use) and organizational environmentalism (e.g. fostering some policies for the sake of environment). Regarding the categorization suggested by Stem and colleagues, individuals' energy conservation behaviors may fall into private sphere behaviors which have been considered as a signal to government, educational and industrial sector pertinent to citizen concerns and consumer preferences (Stem et al., 1999).


Some research studies (Ibtissem, 2010; Menzel & Bogeholz, 2010; Nordlund & Garvill, 2003; Stem et al., 1999) have shown that VBN theory has an exploratory power in various pro- environmental behaviors and some attributes related to specific behaviors such as willingness to make some sacrifices, energy conservation behavior, consumer behavior, commitment to protect biodiversity, willingness to reduce car use. However, beliefs in ascription of responsibility were not included in some studies (i.e Nordlund & Garvill 2003) which did not let an opportunity to test the complete VBN theory. In this aspect, the present study aimed to investigate Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors by testing the full VBN theory. Thus, the following hypotheses could be addressed for this study:



  • Each variable in the causal model proposed to explain Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors is significantly related to the next variable (see Figure 1).

  • Each variable in the causal model proposed to explain Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors is significantly related to the variables further down to the model (see Figure 1).


Method


Sample


Participants in this study were 512 students at Faculty of Education from two public universities located in Central Anatolia Region of Turkey. These faculties as clusters were purposefully selected in that there were some initiations to integrate the vision of ‘sustainability' in the context of campus life and education programs. As a part of sustainable life style, energy saving has been planned to be considered in facultywide elective and must courses, and dissemination activities in these universities. Of the 512 students, 35.5% were enrolled in the early childhood education program, 30.9% were in the elementary science education program, and 27.7% were in the elementary mathematics education program. The rate of the participants pursuing the graduate program under the department of elementary education was 5.9%. These participants did not have any school experience in teaching. Of the participants, 49.6% were junior and 44.5% were senior class students. The average age of the participants was calculated as 22.4 years. Among this sample, 75.0% were female and 25.0% were male.


Instruments


The measuring tool used in the present study covered seven sections. The first section included some items on demographic information such as gender, grade level, age, etc. The other sections focused on the following constructs: energy


conservation behaviors, personal norms, ascription of responsibility, awareness of consequences, New Environmental Paradigm (NEP), and universal values.


Energy Conservation Behaviors


Teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors were assessed by administrating Turkish adapted version of a scale used by Ibtissem (2010). The initial version of the scale covered 11 items, but, with respect to content and construct validity studies, a total of 9 items constituted the final version of the present study domain on energy conservation behaviors. The items rated on a 5-point scale fl’ = never, '2' = rarely, ‘3’ = sometimes, ’4' = frequently, ’5' = always) focused on direct use of electricity and natural gas for heating, lightening, etc. purposes. These items were adapted carefully by considering the daily life facilities that the university students experienced in. For instance, an item used in the original scale "I prefer to shower rather than to take a bath.” was removed from the scale since the appliances in the university dormitories do let the students only to take a shower. The scale items could be exemplified as I switch off the lights if I am the last one leaving the room I wash dirty clothes without prewashing and at low temperatures I leave the electric machines on standby mode and ‘In the winter, I keep the heat on so that I do not have to wear a sweater ’.


Furthermore, in order to eliminate ambiguities, the items were evaluated by three environmental educators and an expert on modem languages. Turkish adapted versions of some items were revised with respect to their responses and reactive comments as well as the results of the pilot study.


To examine the construct validity of the scale, exploratory factor analysis was carried out by using a pilot data set gathered from Turkish university students. Principle component analysis was conducted to make the initial decision about the number of dimensions in the scale. With respect to the scree plot as a statistical criterion (Green & Salkind, 2005), it was concluded that the scale on energy conservation behaviors was uni-dimensional. These results revealed that the items loaded highly on one dimension that overlapped with the one in the original scale. The boundary value of the factor score was set as .30 indicating that an item's factor loading to a component should be at least .30 in this study (Buyukozturk, 2010).


Being a second stage in the construct validity study of the scale, confirmatory factor analysis was carried out by using the present data set. According to the results of the Goodness-of-fit Test, one-factor model was clearly the best fit (Chi-square = 760.184, df = 398, p< .001), pointing out that this scale was uni-dimensional. It was also found out that nine items loaded on one dimension with r values ranging from .43 to .77. However, according to the results of confirmatory factor analysis, two items (‘In the winter, I leave the windows open for long periods of time to let in fresh air ’ and I take a shower in a short period of time (less than 10 min.) ’) did not fit the uni-dimensional model tested on energy conservation behaviors so that they were removed from the scale. The reliability coefficient of Cronbach's alpha was calculated as .72 indicating acceptable internal consistency.


Personal Norms, Ascription of Responsibility, Awareness of Consequences


Teacher candidates participating in the current study were asked to reflect to what extent they agreed with the statements targeting personal norms, ascription of responsibility, and awareness of consequences. A total of 25 items used in previous research studies conducted by Steg, Dreijerink, and Abrahamse (2005) and Ibtissem (2010) were rated on a 5-point Likert type scale (‘ 1' = strongly disagree, '2' = disagree, ‘3' = undecided, ’4' = agree, ’5' = strongly agree). In the context of construct and content validity, the procedures followed on the scale on energy conservation behaviors covering pilot administration were also carried out in the context of these scales.


Examining the construct-related evidences, the results of confirmatory factor analysis reflected uni-dimensionality of personal norms scale with 9 items (e.g ‘I feel personally obliged to save as much energy as possible ‘I feel guilty when I waste energy ‘If I would buy a new washing machine, I would feel morally obliged to buy an energy efficient one I would be a better person if I saved energy ‘I feel obliged to bear the environment and nature in mind in my daily behaviour ’), ascription of responsibility scale with 6 items (e.g'I feel jointly responsible for global warming ‘My contribution to the energy problems is negligible ‘Not only the government and industry are responsible for high energy!
consumption levels, but me too ', ‘In principle, individuals at their own cannot contribute to the reduction of energy problems ).
and awareness of consequences scale with 10 items (e.g ‘Global warming is a problem for society', ‘Energy savings help reduce global warming’, ‘The exhaustion of fossil fuels is a problem ’, ‘The exhaustion of energy> sources is a problem ’, ‘Environmental quality will improve if we use less energy', ‘It is not certain whether global warming is a real problem ’).


Concerning the results of Goodness-of-fit Test, one-dimension model was clearly the best fit for each scale (Personal Norms Scale: Chi- square = 749.172, df = 362, p< .001; Ascription of Responsibility: Chi-square = 812.697, df = 351, p< .001 Awareness of Consequences: Chi- square = 721, df = 349, p< .001), pointing out that these measuring tools were uni-dimensional. It was also found out that nine items loaded on personal norms with r values ranging from .41 to .82,   six        items     loaded   on         ascription of responsibility with r values ranging from .39 to .69, ten items loaded on awareness of consequences with r values ranging from .38 to 75.


The internal consistency of personal norm, ascription of responsibility, and awareness of consequences item sets was found to be .87, .80, and .87, respectively assessed with Cronbach's alpha. These values indicate a good measure of internal consistency for these constructs.


New Environmental Paradigm (NEP)


In order to assess teacher candidates' beliefs on human-nature connectedness, the revised NEP scale (Dunlap et al. 2000) on a 5-point Likert type (ranging from 1 ‘strongly disagree' to 5 ‘strongly agree') was implemented in the present As it was supported by Nunnally (1978), the study. The instrument was adapted into Turkish in a previous research study carried out by Ozsoy (2010). The Cronbach's alpha was calculated as .73 reflecting acceptable internal consistency.


Universal Values


A short version of Schwartz's (1992) universal values scale was used to assess teacher candidates' values guiding their own life. The short version of value scale was adapted from Stem, Dietz, and Guagno (1998) and used by Steg, Dreij erink, and Abrahamse (2005). As supported by Steg, Dreij erink, and Abrahamse (2005), when scholars are interested in egoistic, altruistic and biospheric value orientations, it is reasonable to focus on values pertaining to the dimensions of self-enhancement versus selftranscendence. The participants were asked to rate to what extent 12 values are important as a guiding principle in their lives on a eight-point scale which ranged from 0 ‘not at all important' to 7 ‘of supreme importance'. In case participants were opposed to the value, they were provided with that option (-1). Each value orientation was represented with 4 values; egoistic value orientation with authority, social power, wealth, influential; biospheric value orientation with preventing pollution, respecting the earth, unity with nature, protecting the environment; altruistic value orientation with social justice, helpful, a world at peace, equality. In the context of construct validity, multiple group method (MGM), a type of confirmatory factor analysis was used to decide whether the set of values clustered as it was indicated on theoretical grounds. The results showed that the value statements correlated highest with the value orientation to which they are assigned on theoretical bases (Table 1). basic assumption behind this method is that the loading of value statements into the three value orientations is the highest with the value orientation that they are designated to with respect to theoretical framework. Shown with the corrected correlations in Table 1, it was concluded that the value items structured the



Data Analysis


In order to examine Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors, personal norms, ascribed responsibility, awareness of consequences, beliefs on humannature connectedness, and value orientations, means and standard deviations were calculated through descriptive statistics for the related environmental attributes. Furthermore, a series of multiple linear regression analysis was conducted to determine the significant determinants of variables on energy conservation as proposed by VBN theory (see Figure 1). In this context, a measure of adjusted R~ was reported to represent the proportion of variance in the criterion variable that can be explained by the predictor value orientations as it was suggested by Stem, Dietz, and Guagno (1998) and Steg, Dreijerink, and Abrahamse (2005). For the components of value orientations, Cronbach's alpha values were calculated as .75 for altruistic, .81 for biospheric, and .77 for egoistic values, students were required to complete the instrument via paper and pencil on their own. It took about twenty minutes for the students to complete it.


Results


Teacher candidates participating in this study were asked to reveal their level of agreements to a series of statements about personal norm, ascribed responsibility, awareness of consequences, and NEP. As shown in Table 2, the mean scores on these attributes related to energy conservation indicated that these teacher candidates possessed feelings of moral obligation for energy conservation and of responsibility for causing or preventing adverse consequences of variable (Pallant, 2001). Also, part correlations (semi-partial correlations) designed to eliminate the effect of one variable on two other variables while assessing the correlation between these two variables were also noted. Thus, part correlations could give us an idea about the unique contribution of a predictor variable on the criterion variable. The significance level was set to .05. However, it should be noted that significance level was adjusted by using Bonferroni method which involves dividing alpha level on the basis of the number of analyses conducted. Thus, the new adjusted alpha level became 0.01.



Procedure


The participants of this study were informed about the purpose of the study and the procedure to complete the scales. They also were explained that their identity would be kept anonymous and that the findings of the study would have no effect on their school grades. Furthermore, the










































Mean



Std. Deviation



3.74



.51



4.18



.65



4.35



.52



4.10



.58



3.94



.45



4.53



1.16



6.28



.78



5.97



.88



energy consumption, and were aware of the consequences of energy conservation to the environment, to their own life, and to people rather than own self. Moreover, it was found that the participants had moderate level of favorable general beliefs on human-nature connectedness. Although the participants had favorable feelings of moral obligation and beliefs pertaining to energy conservation, the descriptive results reflected that they did not satisfactorily demonstrate some behaviors on energy conservation. Concerning the teacher candidates' value orientations, the findings showed that altruistic and biospheric values outweigh egoistic values. However, relatively large standard deviation calculated on egoistic values indicated the participants' opinions on egoistic value items were not uniform. In order to test the complete VBN theory in explaining Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors, multiple regression analysis was carried out in this study. Table 3 presents the




Discussion and Implications


The present study aimed to shed light on Turkish elementary teacher candidates' behaviors pertaining to energy conservation by using Value-Belief-Norm Theory. The data gathered in this study supported the idea that these teacher candidates had a feeling of moral obligation, developed a sense of responsibility, and were aware of the consequences to human and non-human living things in the context of energy conservation. As supported by Menzel and Bogeholz (2010), higher personal norms and favorable beliefs in energy conservation could be attributed to direct observation of negative consequences of energy consumption. To satisfy the electricity needs of current and future generations, some governmental and organizational strategies are currently under debate in Turkey. For instance, it has become a controversial issue whether hydroelectric power stations should be used to generate some electricity or not. Reflected by mass media, public agreement is generally on the opposite position of constructing more hydroelectric power stations due to confiscation of home towns or agricultural lands, and environmental degradation. On the other hand, some governmental institutions such as the Ministry of Energy and Natural Resources, and General Directorate of Electrical Power Resources have some initiations to enhance public awareness on energy conservation. Thus, it appeared that these teacher candidates exposed to such dissemination activities and current arguments could develop moral obligation and favorable beliefs on energy conversation. However, these individuals did not strongly demonstrate active engagement in energy conservation. For instance, majority of the students (66.4%) reported that they switch off the lights if they are the last one leaving the room whereas more than half of the students (52.7%) do not frequently prefer to wash their dirty clothes without prewashing and at low temperatures. Similarly, they frequently leave the electric machines on standby mode (51.8%). These results might be due to the fact that the teacher candidates did not possess a certain level of awareness and knowledge based on how some behaviors in our everyday life could make a contribution to energy conservation and environmental quality. At this point, a better understanding of attributes having an impact on energy conservation behaviors comes into prominence for educators and policy makers in formal and non-formal settings.


In order to enhance elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors, it is crucially important to determine the factors which play important roles in explaining such behavioral changes. The results of the current study showed that VBN Theory could successfully explain Turkish elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors to some extent. Each predictor variable in the causal model was found to be significantly associated with the next variable, but value orientations deserves more attention than VBN Theory places on it within the study context.


It was demonstrated that the variation in these teacher candidates' reduction in energy consumption could be accounted by the combination of personal norms, egoistic and biospheric value orientations with a medium effect size. As supported by Schwartz (1977), behaviors to conserve energy might occur in response to personal norms relevant to such behaviors. More specifically, the participants who felt guilty when they wasted energy and felt morally obliged to save as much energy as possible possess higher tendency to demonstrate some behaviors to conserve energy. Considering the supportive evidences provided by some scholars (e.g., Bamberg & Schmidt, 2003; Dervisoglu, Menzel, Soran, & Bogeholz, 2009; Nordlund & Garvill, 2002, 2003; Vining & Ebreo, 1992), personal norms are among the significant predictors of some pro-environmental behaviors such as willingness to reduce in car use, recycling behavior, and biodiversity protection. Interestingly, the present study reflected that egoistic and biospheric value orientations made relatively greater unique contribution to explain energy conservation behaviors than personal norms did. This result indicated that the teacher candidates who attributed higher value to own self (authority, social power, wealth, having an impact on people and events) did demonstrate energy conservation behaviors less. On the other hand, the individuals who valued the quality of environment had higher tendency to reduce energy consumption in their everyday lives. Such results could be also associated with the arguments by Gagnon Thompson and Barton (1994) and Schultz and Zelezny (1999), who pointed out that egoistic value orientations are utilitarian in character. Supported by Nordlund and Garvill (2003) an egoistic individual would be less likely to protect the environment if other human-centered values like material quality of life interfered. Meanwhile, it should be noted that extravagance in energy consumption sometimes points out an individual's bonding to own comfort (e.g keeping the heat on not to wear a sweater). Thus, supported by Steg, Dreij erink, and Abrahamse (2005), the elementary teacher candidates' energy conservation behaviors which may necessitate sacrifies from own comfort require to supress egoistic motives. On the other hand, the teacher candidates with ecocentric worldviews would reduce their energy consumption to protect the environment even if their behaviors required them to sacrifice some material quality of life.


With a large effect size (%56), elementary teacher candidates' personal norms could be determined by the linear combination of biospheric value orientations, awareness of consequences, and ascription of responsibility. The attribute of ascribed responsibility was found to be associated with personal norms although small in magnitude. It implies that the more responsibility teacher candidates feel for causing negative impacts and resolving some problems relevant to energy waste, the more moral obligation they feel to struggle with such challenges. This finding converges with VBN Theory and Norm Activation Theory which postulates that personal norms could be activated in some individuals who believe that they are jointly responsibility for causing an environmental threat and their contribution to environmental problems is significant (Ibtissem, 20100; Stem et al., 1999). On the other hand, similar to the observed results for energy conservation behaviors, biospheric value orientations made the largest unique contribution to the explanation of personal norms which contradicted with the previous research (Dervisoglu et al., 2009; Steg et al., 2005). This finding indicates that teacher candidates who have chosen environmental quality as a guiding principle in their lives feel more obliged to contribute household energy conservation.


As it was hypothesized, an individual's ascribed responsibility for causing or preventing negative consequences of overconsumption in energy could be incented more with an increase in awareness of consequences and strong beliefs on humannature connectedness (NEP). In this study context, these two constructs made similar contributions to explain the elementary teacher candidates' feelings of responsibility on energy conservation. Thus, when compared to the previous studies (Ibtissem, 2010, Steg et.al 2005), general beliefs on human-nature connectedness (NEP) has a more predictive power for their feelings of responsibility one energy conservation. In line with the VBN Theory, NEP was also found to have an explanatory role for the teacher candidates' awareness of adverse consequences of energy consumption. Moreover, the results implied that the individuals who view the environment as worth preserving and respect as a guiding principle of life would be aware of the problems and strong beliefs on human-nature connectedness.


The present study gives educators, policy makers, and administrators some significant clues which could be used to enhance energy conservation behaviors. Considering the role of biospheric value orientations in shaping behaviors and the relevant attributes, teacher candidates could be enrolled in some university-wide activities in outdoor and indoor settings. Supported by Dervisoglu et al. (2009) and Bogeholz (2006), “aesthetic nature experiences' could be integrated into their learning experiences to cultivate their energy conservation behaviors and the relevant factors. In such a learning environment, the teacher candidates could be provided with some opportunities which could let them experience in the beauty of nature. For instance, in Turkey we are still destroying the habitats of birds and some other animals so that they are most likely to become extinct. The major reason is highly linked to construction of dams and hydroelectric power stations in order to satisfy our needs for household and industrial energy. Thus, the beauty of animals and their native environment, unity with nature and alternative strategies to live in harmony with other species could be a better approach to emphasize the human-nature interaction and reinforce biospheric value orientations.


Looking at the associations among energy conservation-related factors, computer-based scaffold learning activities (Nicolaou et al., 2009) could be developed to reinforce energy saving. In this process, the activities may include some simulations which focus on the possible contributions of energy saving actions in our daily life representing the experiences in a natural context. Such activities might also let teacher candidates simulate the challenges of carbon footprint to gain real world experience. In order to reduce energy consumption and greenhouse gas emissions, an energy-saving house was activated also as an online learning environment in Taiwan (Xiao, 2009). Similar online learning processes with some justifications to society and culture could be
pursued in Turkey so that it may unfurl how green energies and sustainable energy consumption work in everyday life to minimize the greenhouse gas emission. In this sense, the teacher candidates might become aware of the consequences of our behaviors, and better grasp the connectedness between human and nonhuman living things which could result in feelings of responsibility and moral obligation for energy conservation.


References



  1. Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall.

  2. Alkis, S. & Ozturk, M. (2007). Sustainable development in opinions of primary student teachers and pre-service teacher education in Turkey, Paper presented at 2007 Symposium of the International Geographical Union - Commission on Geographical Education (IGU - CGE), Lucerne, Switzerland.

  3. Bamberg, S. & Schmidt, S. (2003). Incentives, morality or habit? Predicting students' car use for university routes with the models of Ajzen, Schwartz and Triandis. Environment and Behavior, 35(2), 264-285.

  4. Boldero. J. (1995). The prediction of household recycling of newspapers: The role of attitudes. intentions.   and situational factors. Journal of           Applied Social Psychology. 25, 440-462.

  5. Bogeholz, S. (2006). Nature experience and its importance for environmental knowledge, values and action: Recent (derm an empirical contributions. Environmental Education Research, 12 (l):65-84.

  6. Buyukozturk. S. (2010). Statistical analysis for social sciences: Statistics, research design. SPSS applications and interpretations [Sosyal bilimler icin veri analizi el kitabi: istatistik. ara§tirma deseni. SPSS uygulamalan ve yorum]. Ankara: Pegem Akademi Yayincihk.

  7. Banter. E.H. (2005). The intention-behavior gap: To what degree does Fishbein’s integrated model of behavioral prediction predict whether teachers implement material learned in a professional development workshop? Unpublished PhD. dissertation. The Ohio State University. Columbus. OH.

  8. Dervisoglu, S., Menzel, S., Soran, H. & Bogeholz, S. (2009). Degerler, in an cl ar ve problem algisinin biyolojik ccsitliligi kommaya yonelik kisiscl nonnlara etkisi. Hacettepe University Journal of Education, 37, 50-59.

  9. Dilaver, Z (2009). Residential Electricity Demand for Turkey: A Structural Time Series Analysis. Retrieved on 23-August- 2011, at URL: http://www.aaee.at/2009IAEE/uploads/fullpaper_iaee09/P_140_Dilaver_Zafer%20_29-Jun2009,%2016:51.pdf

  10. Dunlap, R. E. & Van Liere, K. D. (1978). The New Environmental Paradigm. The Journal of Environmental Education, 9(4), 10-19.

  11. Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G. & Jones, R. E. (2000). Measuring endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigma: A revised NEP scale. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 425-442.

  12. Fishbein. M. & Ajzen. I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: an introduction to theory and research. MA. Addison-Wesley.

  13. Gagnon Thompson, S. C. & Barton, M. A. (1994). Ecocentric and anthropocentric attitudes toward the environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14, 149-157.

  14. Green, S., & Salkind, N. (2005). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

  15. Guagnano, G. A., Dietz, T. & Stem, P. C. (1994). Willingness to pay for public goods: A test of the contribution model. Psychological Science, 5(6), 411-415.

  16. Gutierrez Karp, D. (1996). Values and their effect on pro-environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior, 28(1), 111-133.

  17. Haigh, M. (2005). Greening the university curriculum: appraising an international movement, Journal of Geography in Higher Education, 29(1), 31-48

  18. Harland & Wilke (2007). Situational and Personality Factors as Direct or Personal Norm Mediated Predictors of Pro- environmental Behavior: Questions Derived From Norm-activation Theory. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29(4), 323-334.

  19. Hunecke, M., Blobaum, A., Matthies, E. & Hoger, R. (2001). Responsibility and environment, Ecological norm orientation and external factors in the domain of travel mode choice behavior. Environment and Behavior, 33(6), 830-852.

  20. Ibtissem, M. H. (2010). Application of value beliefs norms theory to the energy conservation behaviour. Journal of Sustainable Development, 3(2), 129-139.

  21. Karp, D. G. (1996). Values and their effects on pro-environmental behavior. Environment & Behavior, 28, 111-133.

  22. Kollmus. A.. Agyeman. J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behaviour? Environmental Education Research, 8(3). 239-260.

  23. Kurz. T.. Linden. M. & Sheehy. N. (2007). Attitudinal and community influences on participation in new curb side recycling initiatives in Northern Ireland. Environment and Behavior. 39(3). 367-391.

  24. Menzel, S. & Bogeholz, S. (2010). Values, beliefs and norms that foster Chilean and German pupils' commitment to protect biodiversity. International Journal of Environmental and Science Education, 5( 1), 31-49.

  25. Ministry of Energy and Natural Resources (2011). Retrieved on 23-August-2011, at URL: http://www.enegi.gov.tr/BysWEB/DownloadBelgeServlef?read=db&fileId=167565

  26. Nicolaou, C.T., Korflatis, K., Evagorou, M. & Constantinou, C. (2009). Development of decision-making skills and environmental concern through computer-based, scaffolded learning activities. Environmental Education Research, /5(1), 39-54.

  27. Nordlund, M. A. & Garvill, J. (2002). Value Structures Behind Proenvironmental Behavior. Environment and Behavior, 34, 740-756.

  28. Nordlund, M. A. & Garvill, J. (2003). Effects of values, problem awareness, and personal norm on willingness to reduce personal car use. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 339-347.

  29. Nunnally, JC. (1978) Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

  30. Ozsoy, S. (2010). Effects of eco-school application on elementary school students' environmental literacy levels. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Middle East Technical University, Ankara.

  31. Pallant, J. (2001). SPSS Survival Manual -A step by step guide to data analysis for windows, Buckingham: Open University Press.

  32. Poortinga, W., Steg, L. & Vick, C. (2004). Values, environmental concern, and environmental behavior: A study into household energy use. Environment and Behaviour, 36 (1), 70-93.

  33. Rajecki. D.W. (1982) Attitudes: themes and advances. Sunderland. MA. Sinauer.

  34. Schultz, W. P. & Zelezny, L. (1999). Values as predictors of environmental attitudes: Evidence for consistency across 14 countries. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 19(3), 255-265.

  35. Schultz. P.W.. Gouveia. V.. Cameron. L.. Tankha. G.. Schmuk. P.. & Franek. M. (2005). Values and their relationship to environmental concern and conservation behavior. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 36(4), 457- 455.

  36. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 1-65. New York: Academic Press.

  37. Schwartz, S. H. (1977) Normative influence on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Psychology (Vol. 10. pp. 222-275). New York, San Francisco, London, Academic Press.

  38. Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues 50(4), 19-46.

  39. Schwartz, S. H. & Howard, J. A. (1981). A normative decision-making model of altruism. In P. J. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 189-211), Hillsdale, Erlbaum.

  40. Sener A, & Hazer O. (2007). Values and Sustainable Consumption Behaviour of Women: a Turkish Sample. Sustainable Development, 16, 291-300.

  41. Smithson, M.J. (2003). Confidence Intervals, Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences Series, No. 140. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  42. Sparks. P. & Shepherd. R. (1992). Self-identity and the theory of planned behavior: Assessing the role of identification with "green consumerism. Social Psychology Quarterly. 55. 388-399.

  43. Steg, L., Dreijerink, L. & Abrahamse, W. (2005). Factors Influencing the Acceptability of Energy Policies: Testing VBN Theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25 (4), 415-425.

  44. Stem, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behaviour. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407-424.

  45. Stem, P. C. & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of Social Issues 50(3), 65-84.

  46. Stem, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A. & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-nonn theory of support for social movements: The case of environmental concern. Human Ecology Review, 6, 81-97.

  47. Stem, P. С., T. Dietz T. & Guagnano, G. A. (1998). A brief inventory of values. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 984-1001.

  48. Stem. P. C., Dietz T. & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environment and Behavior, 25(3), 322-348.

  49. Summers, M., Gorney, G. & Childs, A. (2004) Student teachers' conceptions of sustainable development: the starting-points of geographers and scientists, Educational Research, 46(2), 163-182.

  50. Taylor. S. & Todd. P. (1995a). An integrated model of waste management behavior: A test of household recycling and composting intentions. Environment and Behavior. 27. 603-630.

  51. Taylor. S. & Todd. P. (1995b). Decomposition and crossover effects in the theory of planned behavior: A study of consumer adoption intentions. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12. 137-155.

  52. United Nations Development Programme (2010) Adapting to Climate Change. Retrieved on 23-August-2011, at URL: http://www.undp.0rg/envir0mnent/library.shtml

  53. VanVugt, M., Meertens, R. M. & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). Car or public transportation? The role of social orientations in a real-life social dilemma. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 258-278.

  54. Vining, J. & Ebreo, A. (1992). Predicting recycling behavior from global and specific environmental attitudes and changes in recycling opportunities. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1580-1607.

  55. Webster, K. (2004) Rethink, Refuse, Reduce... Education for sustainability in a changing world. Field Studies Publications.

  56. Xiao, T.R. (2009). A study on application and education contents of clean energy from energy conservation house: Taking the clean energy zone in Lohas energy conservation house at the NSTM as an Example. Technology Museum Review, 73(4), 55-69.